Discussion Acculturation And Assimilation Cultural Studies Essay

Chapter 5

The purpose of my study was to explore to what extent Greek-Americans hold attitudes and behaviors for the conservation and intergenerational transmission of their ethnic culture through a cross-sectional analysis of survey on 229 self-identified Greek American members of the Greek Orthodox Metropolis of New Jersey. Overall, the respondents included in the current study had achieved upward mobility – as indicated by their high education attainment and socio-economic status – and they had shown a remarkable high level of preservation of their cultural heritage. They succeeded through their affiliation to the Greek language, the Greek Orthodox Church, the church afternoon schools, their participation to various Greek organizations, their family values, the continuing contact with Greece and their participation in political activities. The results not only provided a quantitative view of the behaviors and attitudes towards culture preservation in the six domains, but also helped us to better understand the acculturation and assimilation process.

Our results support that Greek-Americans included in the current study did not fall into the “straight line assimilation model” as described by Milton Gordon (Gordon 1978). The straight line assimilation theory assumes that the immigrants shed their identifications with their home society and that assimilation into American society is prompted by host society institutions. This model suggests there is essentially but one path through which immigrants can be integrated into the mainstream society. On the contrary our results demonstrated that regardless of the generation, over 90% of the participants identified themselves as either ‘Greek’ or ‘Greek American’ and ~90% of the participants felt a sense of pride and a strong bond with other Greeks and Greek Americans when they attend a Greek heritage event. They are actively involved in the Greek Orthodox Church; they participate in Greek/Hellenic Organizations and support Greek National Interests through donations. All these data suggest that Greek Americans did not shed their identity in order to move up in the American society; rather, they have kept a bicultural identity. Second, the assumption that assimilation into the American society is prompted by the host society institutions does not hold either. For example, the majority of the participants (67%) responded that Greeks living in the United States should try to influence American foreign policy towards Greece and 64% of the participants responded that they had supported (through donations, fund raisers, public expressions of opinion, etc.) the Greek National Interests. This finding coincides with a previous study (Karpathakis, 1999b) revealing that Greek Americans were concerned with Greece’s territorial sovereignty issues and they attempted to influence host society foreign policy regarding Greece. Clearly the Greek cultural identity had affected their assimilation in the American society, which was ignored by Gordon’s paradigm.

In view of the criticisms of the classical assimilation theory by Gordon, Barkan (1995) developed a six stage assimilation model and argued the there has been no one pattern, no one cycle, no one outcome that uniformly encompasses all ethnic experiences. Alba and Nee (2003) re-conceptualized assimilation as an intergenerational process ‘affected not just by social, financial and human capital of immigrant families but also by the ways individuals use these resources with and apart from the existing structure of ethnic networks and institutions’. They argued that assimilation does not preclude retaining elements of ethnic culture. In contrast, Portes and Zhou (1993) proposed the theory of “segmented assimilation”, which asserts that the United States is a stratified and unequal society, and different “segments” of society are available to which immigrants may assimilate. They further argued that total assimilation will put immigrant minorities in vulnerable positions while a strategy of paced, selective assimilation may prove the best course for these groups (Portes and Zhou 1993). Segmented assimilation theory is based on the notion that the living experience in America is very diverse. No single context can apply to all immigrant families and assimilation has varying consequences for immigrants. Critics of segmented assimilation pointed out that the causal link between assimilation into the underclass and development of oppositional cultures among immigrant children is questionable (Xie and Greenman, 2011).

Although these theories have certain limitations, they provide a useful framework for the present study. Our study used a variety of measures of assimilation including spatial concentration, loss of Greek language, socioeconomic status, and intermarriage. According to Waters and Jimenez (2005), these measures are the four primary benchmarks of assimilation and existing literature showing that today’s immigrants are largely assimilating into American society along each of these dimensions.

Spatial concentration, i.e., dissimilarity in spatial distribution and suburbanization is a measure of cultural assimilation and primary and secondary structural assimilation. Spatial assimilation theory asserts that foreign-born residents will choose suburban residential locations after assimilating culturally and socioeconomically (Massey, 1985). Primary structural assimilation occurs when newcomers begin to engage in intimate, small group social interaction with individuals from the dominant group, such as in clubs, social functions, family gatherings, and so on. Secondary structural assimilation occurs when ethnic group members become integrated into the large, impersonal societal groups in the educational, economic and political institutions of the larger society (Marger, 2012). The present study showed that 77% of the participants reside in a suburban area indicating a high degree of structural assimilation among Greek Americans. It is interesting to observe that the First generation (85%) and the Third or beyond generation (80%) had a higher suburbanization rate than the Second generation (71%). Our study also showed that 31% live in a community where many other people of Greek descent live and 56% of the participants live in a community where there are a few people of Greek descent with additional 3% of them reported that there are no people other than their family of Greek descent in their community. The rest 11% of the participants were not sure about their community whether there are any people of Greek descent. Our findings also suggest that living in an area without other people of Greek descent was positively linked to poorer Greek language skills and more negative attitudes and behaviors towards cultural retention in several domains. Thus, community composition played an important role in retention and loss of ethnic culture. Living in a community where there are many people of Greek descent provide the participants more opportunity to network with others, speak Greek language, participating heritage events, which in turn contributed to a higher ethnic pride and feel of belonging and they are more likely to maintain their ethnic culture.

Loss of Greek language is an indicator of acculturation which is in accord with our study. Here, we observed a clear trend of loss of Greek language in the Third or beyond generation as more than half of the participants indicated that their Greek language ability is poor to non-existent. Our findings coincide with Waters and Jimenez’s (2005) three-generation model of language assimilation which stated that the first generation makes some progress in language assimilation but remains dominant in their native tongue, the second generation is bilingual, and the third-generation speaks only English.

Religion is also a measure of acculturation because religion is viewed as a culture construct that occurs and develops within specific cultural contexts (Zinnbauer & Pargament, 2005). As such, religion is likely to influence acculturation by shaping cultural norms, values, behaviors, and attitudes (Yang & Ebaugh, 2001). Similarly, Güngör et al (2012) emphasized that religious reaffirmation is related to cultural values of interdependence, heritage culture maintenance, and ethnic identification.

Socioeconomic status (SES), such as education, occupation status and income, is a measure of secondary structural assimilation. As immigrants begin to venture out into the mainstream educational and employment institutions, their level of interaction with non-ethnics increases and so do the possibilities of engaging with the latter in intimate social interaction within primary social groups (Scott, 2009). Our study suggested high education achievements, high income and more professional fields of occupation among all generations of participants, which showed that Greek Americans have achieved secondary structural assimilation as they enjoy relatively equal access to jobs, political authority and other important opportunities. In other words, they have full participation in all institutional areas of American society.

The majority of the participants had at least college education (77%) and a household income above $50,000 (66%). It is interesting to see that the Second generation had a higher income than both the First and the Third or beyond generation. 41% reported that they work in a professional field including attorneys, medical doctors, accountants, engineers, IT project managers, nurses, family therapists, pharmaceutical sales representatives, etc. 11% of the respondents were in an academic field. Moreover, we observed that 15% of the First generation and 11% of the Second generation had a Ph.D./M.D. degree while not a single Third or beyond generation respondent was found to hold a Ph.D./M.D. degree. Clearly, Greek America should encourage younger generations to achieve higher academic achievements and dedicate themselves to a more diverse professional field including research and teaching.

Intermarriage is an indicator of amalgamation (Waters and Jiménez, 2005). Much of the research has relied on intermarriage as an indicator of assimilation (e.g. Alba, 1981; Alba and Camlin, 1983; Castonguay, 1982; Cohen, 1977). Both interethnic and interfaith marriages were found to be a factor contributing to the process of assimilation. In our study, we not only estimated the intermarriage rate across generations, but we also studied the impact of intermarriage on attitudes and behaviors of preserving Hellenic core values as well as the attitudes towards interethnic and interfaith marriage. The findings are discussed later in further context.

Our study also tested acculturation theories. Berry proposed four modes of acculturation: ‘assimilation’, ‘integration’, ‘separation’ and ‘marginalization’. ‘Assimilation’ refers to the acceptation of the majority’s culture on the expanse of one’s own original minority culture. ‘Separation’ refers to the opposite stance: loyalty toward one’s original minority culture and the rejection of the majority’s culture. ‘Integration’ refers to the adoption of both cultures with an attempt to integrate between them, while ‘marginalization’ refers to distancing from one’s original heritage culture, but this time without assimilating to the majority’s culture. This, results with the individual remaining with no clear cultural identification (Berry, 1997, 2001).

The contribution of the acculturation theory is that it empirically demonstrated the ability to predict desirable or undesirable adjustment outcomes among distinct immigrant groups and members of minority ethnic groups. More specifically, numerous studies found ‘integration’ to be the most successful adaptation strategy by balancing the host country’s culture with the traditional values of one’s own culture origin while marginalization struggled the most to adapt to the dominant society (Berry, 1974, 1980, 1984, 1997, 2003; Berry et al., 2006, Berry, 2010).

A number of factors were found to influence the acculturation process, including, length of time living in the host country (Zheng and Berry, 1991), socio-economic status (Aroian et al, 1998) and social support from the host society (Garcia et al, 2002).

Given the strong Greek culture preservation shown in the current sample, we believe that the participants did not follow assimilation or marginalization strategies in which they would become more alienated toward their own culture. For example, assimilated individuals do not want to keep their identity from their home culture, but would rather take on all of the characteristics of the new culture. On the contrary marginalized individuals don’t want anything to do with either the new culture or the old culture. The results cannot be explained by separation either – where the individuals become alienated toward the host culture and totally separate them from the main society. Our results indicated that 76% of the participants identified themselves as “Greek American” with a socioeconomic status which is above the average in the sampled geographic area (NJ, PA, and VA) based on the 2010 U.S. Census. About half of the participants did not prefer to speak Greek when they were among people who understand Greek (Q15). Third, the majority of the participants did not agree that people of Greek descent should marry people of Greek descent (Q30), and neither did they agree that they would be unhappy if their children married someone who was not a member of the Greek Orthodox Church (Q31), which indicated that they are open to marry non-Greek partners. Our results showed that the participants of this survey had adjusted to the American culture instead of isolating themselves from the host culture. Therefore, the major mode Greek Americans took is integration, by which, the participants embrace both their culture of origin and the society of settlement – thus getting the best of both worlds. The results indicate that the participants have strong positive attitudes and behaviors towards preserving the Greek heritage. For example, they thought it is important for their children and people of Greek ancestry to speak Greek, they routinely attended worship services at the Greek Orthodox Church, they participated in Greek organizations like AHEPA and they attended Greek heritage events with a sense of pride and bond with other Greeks.

All these characteristics demonstrated that the Greek-Americans had amply preserved their culture of origin and they gradually integrated themselves into the main society. In addition, the participants showed a more democratic attitude which has been influenced by their integration into the American culture. For example, the majority of the Second and Third or beyond generations disagreed that the father should have the final say in most important decisions.

Overall, we identified a shift from Greek culture values to shared Greek-American values through generations. Such shift may reflect the need to utilize the best of both cultures. As Karpathakis (1999b) argued that with economic and cultural globalization, persons with bi-national identity are increasingly seen by the mainstream as assets. Therefore, it is more advantageous for Greek Americans to utilize the best of both worlds by following an “integration” approach to adaptation. Bicultural identification was also linked to immigrants’ engagement in their “host” societies. When immigrants perform bicultural identities they are more likely to be involved in the political life of their country of origin (Simon and Ruhs, 2008) and also have more opportunities to engage in political institutions within the host society (Huo and Molina, 2006).

A new version of the “straight line model” has come into circulation as a construct for explaining the participation of White ethnic group members in cultural heritage activities. First proposed by Gans in 1979, the concept of “symbolic ethnicity” denotes a new stage in the assimilation of middle-class, suburban Whites into an inclusive, Pan-European or Pan-White identity in which ethnicity is no longer the basis for collective action or the transmission of distinctive ethnic cultures across generations. As Gans (2009) recently stated:

Symbolic ethnicity proposes the rejection of or a departure from active ethnicity: from participation in ethnic groups and in ethnic culture. It hypothesizes a passive ethnicity, involving the temporary and periodic expression of feelings about or toward the ethnic group or culture through material and non-material symbols. Symbolic ethnicity can even be a leisure time activity that does not interfere with the economic, social and other imperatives of everyday life (p.123).

Read also  Attractions And Culture Of Southall Cultural Studies Essay

Rather than functioning as a structural factor that shapes access to social networks or as a social identity entwined with self-conceptions, in this model, ethnicity is recast into the equivalent of an avocation or hobby that middle-class White Americans periodically use as a means of feeling good about them. According to Gans, White ethnic identity may have a transient influence on individual self-esteem, but it no longer serves as a source of values, group cohesion, or as a determinant of behavior. In addition, as Waters (1990, 2000, 2009) has argued in her work on ethnic options, among White Americans of mixed ancestry, individuals can and do choose situational-contingent ethnic identities. The implications of symbolic ethnicity for Greek Americans in general and particularly for those who reside in middle-class suburban communities are substantial. At least, some members of this ethnic group may embrace a superficial Greek identity without the risk of incurring liabilities that were once attached to being viewed as a “non-White” race subordinate to the Anglo-Saxon core culture. In contemporary American society, then, the “costs” of being identifiably Greek are negligible but the affiliating bonds that join Greek Americans into a distinct group may weaken within and across generations.

We observed that 15 participants from our respondents, who are either Second or Third or beyond generations, had identified themselves as ‘American’. They have probably assimilated more deeply into the American culture; yet, the majority of them responded that they actively participated in the Greek heritage events. These participants tried to take the positive images of their ethnicity while not having to deal with the real social cost of being ethnic, which is a good indication of symbolic ethnicity. Our study also revealed that Greek Americans involved various symbols of ethnicity in their daily life such as eating ethnic food, listen to Greek music, dance Greek dances, read ethnic newspapers, etc. I am in agreement with the findings of Alexiou (1993), which suggested that ethnic identification of Greek Americans does not weaken as generation becomes more removed from their immigrant ancestors, but rather becomes symbolic without structural commitments to ethnic ties.

Patterns of transmission in core values of Hellenic culture

Our study examined the attitudes and behaviors of the Greek American retention of six core values of Hellenic culture. These attitudes are often referred as acculturation orientations and viewed as mediators or moderators between acculturation conditions and acculturation outcomes, while acculturation behaviors can be assumed to be associated to short- term acculturation outcomes (Arends-Tóth & van de Vijver, 2006). As defined by Omi and Winant (1994), ethnicity comprises a culture that includes religion, language, nationality and political identifications. Alba and Nee (2003) viewed ethnicity as a social boundary or distinction that individuals make in their everyday lives that shapes their action and mental orientation toward others. This distinction is embedded in a variety of cultural and social norms, values, and beliefs (p. 11). Similarly, Isajiw (1992) suggested that ethnic identity can be divided into two basic aspects: external and internal. Where external aspects refer to observable behavior patterns, such as language, family, friendship, participation in ethnic/institutional and associational organization and participation in functions sponsored by ethnic organizations, internal aspects refer to images, ideas, attitudes and feelings about their own ethnicity.

Through my own experiences as an active member of Greek America and my extensive research from previous studies, I observed that Greek culture values mainly lie in six domains: (a) Greek language, (b) the Greek Orthodox Church, (c) Family cultural orientation and values, (d) Greek cultural activities and organization membership, (e) Continuing contact with Greece and/or Cyprus, and (f) Political activity. Greek language is an important factor reinforcing ethnic identity. The Greek Orthodox Church is a vibrant and indispensable component of Greek ethnicity by providing an extensive range of religious, educational and social activities and the major sponsor of Greek Heritage festivals. Language and religion have been most frequently studied as acculturation measures (Harris & Verven, 1996). Family cultural orientation and values are also of great importance as children acquire their sense of belonging through their family. The Greek cultural activities and organization offer opportunities to share the experiences and continuing contact with Greece. Finally, ethnic political involvement was viewed as an indicator of assimilation, mobility and acculturation. As Parenti (1967) claimed, the political acculturation of the ethnic proceeds hand in hand with general cultural adaptation to American life and that it is largely completed by the advent of the second generation. These core values can also be used to understand the basis of ethnicity and culture.

In this study, I investigated the cultural retention and loss in each domain. In general, ethnic behaviors are strongest or most apparent among the generations closest to the immigrant experience and become weaker or less apparent among those further away (Alba 1990). Later generation Americans are likely to adopt American cultural ways and modify parental ways so that the original values and behaviors characteristic of the immigrant group become altered or nonexistent. Therefore each successive generation that replaces the previous generation will be less ethnic-identified and the group as a whole will also become less ethnic-identified than their predecessors (Alba 1995). All six domains of the Greek ethnic culture examined in this study, to some extent, followed this general trend. However some of the cultural values experienced considerable reduction from one generation to the next, while other values experienced only minor reductions or modifications. Study suggests that it may not be until the third or fourth generations that families fully acculturate to the host culture (Kelley and Tseng, 1992). Moreover, further generations tend to acculturate more readily than their parents due to the fact that the home culture values are less established among them and they have greater exposure to the host culture through education and contact with non immigrant peers (Phinneey, 1990). This suggests that there might be disparity in cultural transmission across generations.

In our case, we are making an attempt to be more concise by identifying four major patterns of transmission in core values of Hellenic culture (Figure 15).

First, the preservation of Greek language is progressively weakened from one generation to the next. Significant differences were observed across all three generations on self assessed Greek language skills, “whether they speak Greek when at home”, “preference to speak Greek when among people who understand Greek” and opinion on “whether people of Greek ancestry should be able to speak Greek”. Our findings coincide with previous studies (e.g. Costantakos 1982, Demos 1988). Costantakos (1982) analyzed a survey of 211 Greek-Americans living in an unspecified metropolitan area. The study indicated the same pattern of behavioral changes, while attitudes towards retention of the Greek language were positive. In our study, the attitudes towards Greek language preservation were measured by two questions: “whether it is important that my children are able to speak Greek” and “whether it is important for people of Greek ancestry to speak Greek”. The attitudes were found to be positive among the First and Second generation, but not among the Third or beyond generation. To be more specific, 92% of the First generation and 83% of the Second generation supported that it is important for their children to be able to understand and speak Greek, while only 48% of the Third or beyond generation thought so. 87% of the First generation and 70% of the Second generation supported that ‘People of Greek ancestry who live in the United States should be able to understand and speak Greek’, while only 48% of the Third or beyond generation agreed.. Demos (1988) analyzed questionnaires collected from 583 Greek Americans from two Greek Orthodox churches, one in Minneapolis, Minnesota, and the other in Baltimore, Maryland. The study showed that the focus of Greek ethnicity was shifting away from mother tongue maintenance through forces of assimilation and ethnic intermarriage. The study also revealed that the Greek language persisted as a characteristic of the Greek Orthodox Americans and that visits to Greece represent a major way of maintaining the Greek language. In the current study, the self assessed Greek language skills were found to be moderately correlated to the question “Whether the participant has traveled to Greece”, which coincides with the findings in Demos (1988) that visits to Greece represent a major way of maintaining the Greek language.

The current study seems to well fit the model of Anglicization which was initially formulated by the sociolinguists Joshua Fishman (1972, 1980) and Calvin Veltman (1983). The model described that the process occurs in the following ways: some individuals of the immigrant generation learn English, but they generally prefer to speak their native language, especially at home. Thus, their children usually grow up as bilinguals, but many of them prefer English, even in conversing with their immigrant parents (Lopez 1996). The second generation generally speaks English at home when its members establish their own households and rear children. Consequently, by the third generation, the prevalent pattern is English monolingualism and knowledge of the mother tongue for most ethnics is fragmentary at best. Similarly, Portes and Schauffler (1994) argued that regardless of where immigrants live, English will replace the native language within two or three generations unless bilingualism is promoted. In order for second or beyond generation youth to maintain their parents’ native language they must be motivated to use it and provided with opportunities to use this language in places beyond the household, such as school and the broader community.

The second pattern of intergenerational change observed, was connected to the path of the Greek Orthodox Church and the Greek Cultural Activities with minimal reduction. The Greek Orthodox Church assumed a leading role in the preservation of Greek culture in the United States since it hosts both religious and ethnic social events. Researchers who have studied the Greek Orthodox Church and the cultural activities that it sponsors (Alex, 2007; Charalambous, 2004; Demos, 1989, Tsimpouki, 2002) affirmed that it has served as a bulwark of ethnic cultural identity. In the current study, a significant higher proportion of subjects responded to all attitude and most of the behavior questions positively among all three generations and no significant difference was observed among the three generations for most of the questions. Our findings also supported that regardless of the generation status, the participants in our study had very strong positive attitudes towards the Greek Orthodox Church. The vast majority of the participants agreed that it is important for their children and people of Greek ancestry to participant and belong to the Greek Orthodox Church and at least some part of Sunday worship should be conducted in Greek. Yet, their actual behaviors in terms of their participation of the Greek Orthodox Church showed minor decline across generations.

Significant difference was only found between the First and Third generation in terms of “whether their children attend or attended an afternoon Greek school” and “whether part of the Sunday worship should be conducted in Greek”. The results demonstrated that Greek Americans continued to rely on the Orthodox Church to reinforce their ethnic identities. Furthermore, our study examined the perception of respondents on their understanding of the Orthodox Faith. Our religious core beliefs help us face the problems of life and prepare us for salvation as well as keep our Hellenic identity. We must have a sufficient knowledge and understanding of orthodoxy and share this faith with others. Our results showed that the vast majority (94%) of the respondents – regardless of generation – believed that they had good or very good understanding of the Orthodox Faith, the Divine Liturgy and the Holy Sacraments. The Divine Liturgy is the most significant ancient Christian service and the center of the inspiration of the first Christians in their communion with God and with one another (Mastrantonis, 2004). The Divine Liturgy is the central worship service of the Orthodox Church which is celebrated every Sunday morning and on all Holy Days. The Liturgy is also the means by which we achieve union with Jesus Christ and unity with each other through the Sacrament of the Holy Communion. The Holy Sacraments are composed of prayers, hymns, scripture lessons, gestures and processions. Most of the Sacraments use a portion of the material of creation as an outward and visible sign of God’s revelation (Fitzgerald, 2004).

In our study, only six respondents admitted that they had poor or non-existent understanding of the Orthodox Faith and the majority of these six respondents also had poor ability to understand and speak Greek. This is an important issue as both Greek America and its Orthodox population underwent significant generational transformations during1940 to1990. As a result, religious faith and ethnic identity, once seen as inseparable, were increasingly less understood as such by the socially mobile, geographically dispersed, English-speaking second, third or beyond generations of Orthodox faithful in America, not to mention an ever-increasing number of converts (Stokoe and Kishkovsky, 1995).

The Greek Archdiocese, for whom the very definition of Greek identity comes from the Greek language, has undergone continuous debate on the issue since 1962 (Stokoe and Kishkovsky, 1995). In 1964, the Clergy-Laity Congress allowed certain readings and prayers in the liturgy to be repeated in English. In the important 20th Clergy-Laity Congress of 1970, following the personal appeal of Archbishop Iakovos, an English liturgy was permitted. Today, most Orthodox churches do some and in many cases most of their services in English. This policy provides an opportunity for the second and third or beyond generations to comprehend more proficiently the Orthodox Faith. Another important issue we should point out is that orthodoxy itself is not static; it is constantly under pressure to be assimilated or integrated – especially in an Anglo Saxon Protestant society. According to our findings, the Greek Orthodox Church has undergone substantial integration. As a religion and a cultural heritage, the Orthodox Church can also bring the best of both worlds and to blend and orchestrate into a whole, yet not lose essence or identity (Nicozisin, 1993).

Although our results show that the majority of our respondents have a good or very good understanding of the Greek Orthodox Faith – there might be a dichotomy of opinion as to the accuracy of these findings – being that the personal feelings of the respondents could be quite subjective. My personal experience indicates that the majority of the churchgoers – especially the third generation and beyond – are lacking significantly in their understanding of the Greek Orthodox worship and its meaning in depth. One reason to my claim is the fact that the Second and Third or beyond generation churchgoers do not comprehend the original language of the Holy Bible. The other reason is that religion and/or the history of the Greek Orthodox Church and its Holy Tradition is not taught consistently and systematically in our places of worship. As I have visited many churches as a worshiper and/or a guest speaker – it has been evident to me that although the attitudes of the younger parishioners were mostly positive regarding their attendance – their substantive understanding of the Orthodox Faith was rather limited. Since the GOC plays such an important role in the viability of Greek America and its assumed role as a robust pillar of ethnic identity – we highly recommend an on-going and consistent teaching of the Orthodox Christian Faith and its Holy Sacraments to every generation of Greek Americans – especially the new converts to the Orthodox Faith through intermarriage.

Read also  Values And Beliefs Influenced By Diamond Industry Cultural Studies Essay

A similar pattern was also shown in Greek cultural activities. Positive behaviors or attitudes were observed for all questions and no significance was observed among the three generations for almost all questions including ‘the active attendance and support to Greek heritage events’, ‘feeling a sense of pride when attending Greek heritage events’, and ‘studying of Greek history’. The only significant difference observed was “dancing Greek dances” between the Second and Third or beyond generations. All three generations actively attend Greek heritage events and they felt a sense of pride and strong bond with other Greeks and Greek Americans when attending such heritage events.

Hofstede (1980) suggested that traditional Greek culture is a collectivist culture. Greeks are mainly occupied with activities associated with their in-groups like family, friends, work-group, religious group and communities. Greek cultural activities such as Greek festivals and heritage events provide a “unique experience” for Greeks to come together and share their cultural values like Greek food, music, dance, arts, crafts, etc., and help them to develop a sense of national identity and pride of heritage. The Greek cultural activities to a great extent are linked to the Greek Orthodox Church since the church sponsored the majority of those heritage activities. The present findings coincide with the previous study of Kopan (1989), which demonstrated that the American-born generations favored preservation of religious and cultural legacies and they were oppressed by the weight of their own history, thus the continuity of their culture is too strong for alteration.

The third pattern of change observed – is that more similarities were shared by the First and Second generation while the Third or beyond generation deviated more from the First and Second generation. This pattern applies to behaviors that contribute to the conservation of ‘Family Cultural Orientation and Values’ and ‘On-going contact with Greece’. There are two questions measuring the behaviors that contribute to conservation of family cultural orientation and value. One question asked “whether the participants honor and celebrate Greek heritage in their families”, the other one asked “whether they are in frequent contact with family members who do not live in their homes”. Although all three generations responded to both questions positively, the Third or beyond generation showed significant difference from the First and Second generation, while the First and Second generations are very similar in their responses. The deviation of the Third or beyond generation from First and Second generation in family cultural values, can be explained by the significantly higher rate of intermarriage found in the Third or beyond generation than the First and Second generation, since multiple ancestry usually leads to weaker ethnic identity and more limited exposure to ethnic culture as described by Alba (1995).

Study shows that the family is the primary socialization agent on children within a cultural context with parental attitudes contributing to the structure of their ethnic identity (Super and Harkness, 1997). It was also found that perceived parental attitudes accounted for about 20% of the variation of their children’ mode of acculturation (Sam, 1995). Parental attitudes were measured by seven questions in our study covering the perceived importance of their children to understand and speak Greek, to have college education, to participate in the Greek Orthodox Church, to give more freedom to their sons than to their daughters, to marry someone who is not a member of the Greek Orthodox Church, to take care of their parents when they get older, and to live on their own by the time they are 21 years of age. It appeared that all generations of the participants strongly supported the statements that their children should have a college degree and should participate in the Greek Orthodox Church while rejecting the idea of giving more freedom to their sons than to their daughters or letting children live on their own by the time they are 21 years of age. This revealed differences between Hellenic culture and mainstream American values. A person past “college age” who lived with their parents was viewed negatively in American culture. On the other hand, this finding may be also influenced by recent change in the U.S. society with increases in age at which young Americans leave their parental home due to the economic slowdown. Today it is not uncommon for children to live with their parents until their mid-twenties, which are mostly attributed to rising living costs that far exceed those in decades past. According to a new Pew Research poll (Parker, 2012), 21.6% of Americans ages 25 to 34 now live in multigenerational households. The figure has risen steadily since 1980, when it measured at just 11%, and it spiked, unsurprisingly, starting in 2007.The First generation strongly supported that their children should understand and speak Greek and their children are expected to take care of their parents when they get older. They are neutral when it comes to the interethnic and interfaith marriage. The Second and Third or beyond generation are both very open to the idea of interethnic and interfaith marriage. They varied in the perceived importance for their children to speak and understand Greek. The majority of the Second generation (83%) still think it is important while more than half (53%) of the Third or beyond generation did not think so. These findings suggest that parental attitudes towards gender difference, Greek language, and intermarriage had changed dramatically while they still valued the Greek Orthodox Church and educational achievement greatly. This yielded practical implications because the attitudes of the Third or beyond generation will influence how their children-the further generations of Greek America-can maintain their cultural core values.

For Greek Americans, the family is a focus of life (Demos, 1989). The movement from a rural patriarchal life in Greece to an urban existence in the New Greek World has meant that the nature of Greek ethnicity has had to change in order for it to be regained. Demos further argued that it is difficult to maintain such traditional family values with increased exogamy and decreased ability in ethnic mother-tongue, which showed agreement with the present study. Furthermore, another previous study showed that the family has produced and has been the most significant place where the sense of “Greekness” is transferred between generations and is often linked with ongoing relationships with the country of origin (Tsolidis 1995). This again coincides with the findings of our study as the same pattern was observed in the domain ‘Family Cultural Orientation and Values’ and ‘On-going contact with Greece’. Our results support the argument that the Second generation is more likely to follow traditional expectation of maintaining an ethnicity at the family level – while the erosion of traditional features of ethnicity at home occurs in the Third or beyond generation.

The third pattern was also observed in two out of the three behavior questions in section ‘Continuing contact with Greece and/or Cyprus’. According to Moskos (1989) “one cardinal feature of Greek-American ethnicity is the trip back to the old country”. He further suggested that perhaps the majority of Greek Americans have visited the ancestral homeland at least once and regular trips to Greece provided not only a tie for family reunions but also the opportunity to bolster the Greek language competence of their children. Visiting the homeland allows them to have continuing bonds with their roots and culture.

Demos also (1988) suggested that visits to the homeland specifically contributed to the retention of the Greek language. In our study, the vast majority of the respondents – regardless of the generation – have traveled to Greece for at least once – and relatives and/or friends from Greece or Cyprus have visited them in the United States. Yet, the Third or beyond generation showed significantly less frequency in traveling to Greece and inviting friends and relatives from Greece or Cyprus to their home in the United States than the First and Second generations. This indicates that the Third or beyond generation had less connections with Greece or Cyprus, which in turn enhanced the cultural gap between the Third or beyond generation from their parental generations. On the contrary the Second generation demonstrated stronger cultural awareness and practice than the Third or beyond, in aspects which were mostly influenced at an individual or family level. The dramatic decrease in continuing contact with Greece and/or Cyprus in the Third or beyond generation could be explained by the fact that the Third or beyond generation had no relatives or friends who still live in Greece or Cyprus. Moreover, the significantly higher rate of mixed marriages in the Third or beyond generation may also contribute to the fact that they do not have as many relatives in Greece/Cyprus. Despite lacking ties to Greece or Cyprus, the Third generation overwhelmingly expressed an interest in traveling to Greece as 88% of them supported that “People of Greek descent should try to visit Greece or Cyprus at least once”. Traveling back to the “homeland” not only provides a time for family reunion, but also opportunity to speak Greek and experience the glorious Hellenic heritage. Thus, programs such as summer language and culture courses, exchange programs for youth and college students and continuing education programs for adults should be made more available for young Greek Americans as a very effective way to preserve their cultural identity.

The fourth pattern of change observed – is that more similarities are shared by the Second and Third or beyond generation as they deviate significantly from the First generation. This pattern was seen in organizational memberships and political activities. The question on organizational membership asked “whether participants are in one or more “Greek” or “Hellenic” organizations outside of the Greek Orthodox Church”. Significantly higher proportion of participants answered “Yes” in the First generation, while only about half of the Second and Third or beyond generation answered “Yes”. Studies suggest that the cultural traditions are maintained through formations of ethnic organizations (Bonus 2000, Posadas 1999). Ethnic organizations not only provide us a plethora of opportunities to network with others of similar ethnic backgrounds, but also invoke cultural affinity and political opportunities (Moya, 2005). There are many ethnic organizations our participants have engaged in, such as the American Hellenic Educational Progressive Association (AHEPA), Daughters of Penelope (DOP), the Council of Hellenes Abroad (SAE), American Hellenic Institute (AHI), various Orthodox Churches, and professional organizations, like the Hellenic Dancers of NJ, and the Philadelphia Hellenic Lawyers Association – just to mention a few. These organizations play an important role in promoting Hellenism. For example, AHEPA, which was founded in 1922, is the most frequently joined organization by all generations.

We observed a dramatic decline in ethnic organization membership participation among generations. About half of the First generation belonged to an ethnic organization, while less than one third of the Second generation belonged to an ethnic organization. The participation of ethnic organization membership was further dropped to 10% in the Third or beyond generation. Possible explanations are 1) The Second and Third or beyond generation may have achieved upward social mobility to a greater extent than the First generation, which has enabled them to have a greater opportunity to participate in the general organizations open to all ethnicities in the U.S. society; 2) The Second and Third or beyond generation has weaker ethnic-group ties which might be due to the fact that they have stronger ethnic language barriers.

The same trend was observed in four out of five questions in the political activities section. A number of models have been constructed to describe the cross-generational political attitudes and behaviors among immigrants and their descendants. Marcus Lee Hansen developed an explicitly generational model of attachment to the United States (1938), which states that “What the son wishes to forget the grandson wishes to remember”. Hansen predicts a greater loyalty to U.S. values and institutions in the second generation and a return to the values of the immigrant generation in the third generation. Dahl (1961) developed a model which came closest to the notion of assimilation that sees each generation coming closer to being indistinguishable from Americans.

Contrary to Dahl’s findings, Wolfinger (1965) suggests that voting for co-ethnics increases from the first to second generation. The segmented assimilation scholars (Portes and Zhou, 1993; Portes and Rumbaut 2001) also disagreed with Dahl and argued that attitudinal change across generations could steadily bring the ethnic populations closer to or further away from those of populations with earlier ancestries. All these theories offer somewhat different expectations for the role of generation in political attitudes. Empirical evidence from a study conducted on Mexican American (Lamare, 1982) showed that there is a linear progression of political integration through the first and second generations while the third generation showed declining level of commitment to the American political community.

These mixed results complicate the interpretation of our study. Nonetheless, our findings provided valuable empirical evidence of intergenerational change in political attitudes and activities. In particular, we observed a dramatic decrease in support of political candidates who are of Greek descent or who support Greek national issues in the Second and Third or beyond generation. In fact, only first generation Greek Americans showed significantly positive attitudes towards voting for candidates of Greek decent or candidates supporting Greek national issues. The driving forces of this pattern may likely derive from the similarities in the process of socialization of the Second and Third or beyond generations which are quite different than the First generation. Another important point to realize is that such a generation gap in political involvement may not only be specific for Greek America but it can also apply to other ethnicities in the United States at large. Studies showed that Americans growing up in recent decades vote less often than their elders and pay less attention to politics (Bennett and Craig 1997; Keeter et al. 2003).

Our results of the same intergenerational pattern – found in organizational membership and political activities – are also in agreement with previous study (Nelson, 1982). Nelson (1982) studied six ethnic groups in New York and found that ethnic group membership is the best predictor of political attitudes and participation.

Read also  Understanding Cultural And Ethnic Identities

Our study also demonstrated that in general, the behaviors and attitudes of the participants towards each domain are in good agreement. However for some domains, such as, the Greek Orthodox Church and cultural activities, both retention of behaviors and attitudes are very high, while in other domains, either retention of attitudes has a higher intensity than retention of behaviors – such as the language domain – or retention of behaviors has a higher intensity than retention of attitudes, such as family values. In the Greek Orthodox Church domain, the majority of the participants agreed that it is important for people of Greek ancestry (77%) to attend the services and they actually attended by 60%. In the cultural activity domain, over three-fourths of the participants agreed that ‘people of Greek descent should actively support Greek heritage events’ and they felt ‘a sense of pride and strong bond when attending these events’, which showed high intensity of cultural retention internally. In practice, over 75% of them actively participated in these Greek heritage events.

In the language domain, the attitudes towards ethnic identity retention are stronger than the actual behaviors. Over 70% of the participants in our study agreed that it is important that their children and people of Greek ancestry are able to understand and speak Greek. In practice, only 26% speak the Greek language more frequently than English at home. In ‘Family Cultural Orientation and Values’ domain, the behaviors of retention of ethnicity are very strong based on the fact that over 90% of the participants honor and celebrate Greek heritage within their families and they are in frequent contact with family members who do not live in their home. The attitudes towards retention of ethnicity in the same domain are very diverse. It is shown that the participants preserved certain values such as expecting their children to have a college degree and to take care of their parents when they get older. However, family traditions such as ‘father is expected to have the final say’ and ‘sons should be given more freedom than daughters’ have experienced a dramatic decline across generations.

Isajiw (1993) asserted that the retention of ethnic identity from one generation to another does not necessarily mean retention of both behaviors and attitudes or retention of different domains in the same degree. Some components may be retained more than others and some may not be regained at all. Therefore, we should not assume that the ethnic identity retained by the third generation is the same type or form of identity as that regained by the first or the second generation. He further distinguishes various forms of ethnic identity. For example, a high level of retention of practice of ethnic traditions accompanied by a low level of such subjective components as feelings of group obligation – is ‘ritualistic ethnic identity’. On the other hand, a high intensity of feelings of group obligation accompanied by a low level of practice of traditions – is ‘ideological identity’. Negative images of one’s own ethnic group, accompanied by a high degree of awareness of one’s ethnic ancestry – is a ‘rebelling identity’. Positive images of one’s ancestral group accompanied by a practice of highly selected traditions by the third generation – is an ‘ethnic rediscovery’. In this sense, our study demonstrated that the Greek Americans either have ‘ideological identity’ – such as in the language domain – or ‘identity rediscovery’ – such as in the ‘cultural activities’ domain.

To summarize, Greek America is changing its identity from one generation to another as Greek identity has different meanings to different generations. On one hand, all generations maintain those traditions which are central to their lives, such as the Orthodox Church and the sense of pride of being Greek. On the other hand, ability to speak Greek has experienced the most significant decline as the younger generations may not perceive it is as important as the First generation. These results are relevant to the theory of deconstruction and reconstruction of ethnic identity (Isajiw, 1993). Isajiw suggested that in a culturally pluralistic society like the United States, there is a large number and a great diversity of ethnicities with one “mainstream” socio-economic structure as the legitimate locus of aspirations for all groups. Thus, pressures are exerted on all the aspects of ethnicity – to adapt. The process of deconstruction and reconstruction is the mechanism through which this adaptation is achieved. Deconstruction consists of some objective aspects of ethnic identity losing their meaning and use or retaining the meaning but no longer used. Although some aspects of one’s identity may be abandoned and patterns from different cultural sources be acquired and become more meaningful, other aspects may be retained and continue to have significance. Similarly, at a certain point, an individual’s ethnic background or group experience may acquire new meaning and be objectified in fresh, visible ethnic patterns; this is the process of identity reconstruction. It is likely that over the generations, some highly selective old patterns will be recovered and given new significance. These two processes are the ways in which individuals in their everyday living come to modify the meaning of their own identity and change at least some of their ethnic behavior patterns without necessarily divesting themselves of all of them. For example, low retention and little use of ethnic language as mother tongue and low incidence of feelings of obligation to marry within one’s own ethnic group, would be indicators of ethnic identity deconstruction. A high incidence of ethnic food consumption, a high incidence of some knowledge of ethnic language expressions and words – are indicators of ethnic identity reconstruction.

In our study, although a high overall retention was achieved, we saw examples of both reconstruction and deconstruction of ethnic identities, which reflect the changing nature of ethnic identity. For example, ‘the family values domain’ clearly demonstrated ‘ethnic deconstruction’ specifically in terms of the disagreement that the father has the final ‘say’ and the son should have more freedom than the daughter. The most obvious phenomenon of ‘ethnic reconstruction’ we find in the transformation of the Greek Orthodox Church from a pure religious to a ‘multifunctional’ ethno-religious institution in the Hellenic diaspora.

Factors contributing to the difficulties of preserving Greek ethnicity

Clearly, we can see that the broad forces that have contributed to intergenerational erosion in Greek American ethnic identity in the past are still at work. At least, four inter-related factors have contributed to the difficulties that Greek Americans have encountered in their efforts to preserve their distinctive ethnicity.

First, despite concerted efforts and an express desire to transmit knowledge of the Greek language to their children, there has been a continuing intergenerational loss of Greek language fluency and usage (Costantakos, 1982: Demos, 1988; Panagakos, 2000). English had replaced Greek in most Greek Orthodox Churches, professional Greek-American organizations and other ethnic organizations, especially in those organizations mostly composed of second and third generations (Kourvetaris, 1999a). According to the American Community Survey Reports (Shin and Kominski, 2010), Greek, along with Italian, Yiddish, German and Polish, was spoken at home by fewer individuals in the United States in 2007 than in 1980. In 1980, 401,443 people spoke Greek at home, while in 2007, only 329,825 people spoke Greek at home, which is a large proportionate decrease (17.8%).

Language has been frequently cited as a contributor to ethnic identity (Hurtado and Gurin, 1995; Miller and Hoogstra, 1992). A number of factors were revealed as contributing factors to the retention of mother tongue, including residence patterns, religion, school, festival, homeland, labor, marriage, and etc. Demos (1988) found that a higher density of ethnic population provides a greater opportunity to speak the mother tongue and increases the likelihood that it will be retained. Religious ideology and religious practice from

the homeland may be closely associated with the mother tongue (Schrauf, 1999). Study shows that no matter how long an immigrant has lived in the country of adoption, he or she will continue to pray interiorly in the mother tongue (Fitouri, 1983). Ethnic Festivals provide a context in which the mother tongue would naturally be spoken. The same phenomenon is observed in ethnic communities where traditional occupations predominate and use of the mother tongue may be frequent (Schrauf, 1999). Nonetheless, two factors, predominately at work, were seen to accelerate the loss of Greek mother tongue, which are – English-Only Language Policies in public schools and the growing popularity of the Spanish-English bilingualism.

1) English-Only Language Policies in public schools – Although U.S. has been characterized by diverse ethnicities since the country was founded, English has been considered as the common language with a “mainstream” value (Crawford, 1995). The “English-only” movement promoted the enactment of legislation designed to restrict the use of other languages besides English by government entities. Declaring English as the official language evokes arguments over many controversial issues. Nonetheless, 31 states of the United States have enacted laws making English their official language. As a result, many states adopted programs that used English as the exclusive instructional language and set as the primary goal the development of children’s literacy and academic skills in English (Beykont, 1994, 1997; Crawford, 1995). Language minority students were not given any special educational provisions and were instructed in mainstream classrooms alongside native speakers of English. The federal bilingual law mandated that school districts take some type of affirmative educational measure to ensure equal educational opportunity for language minority students and stated a preference for native language instruction, but it did not define exactly what an optimal program should look like (Beykont, 2002). In fact, the vast majority of the U.S. bilingual programs are ‘early-exit transitional programs’ that are designed to transition students into all-English classes as quickly as possible and leave the home language as well as ethnic culture behind. No emphasis was placed on maintaining and developing students’ native language skills or fostering ethnic culture maintenance throughout the school years. By contrast, some of the bilingual education, known as ‘late-exit transitional programs’ and ‘maintenance programs’, do develop the home language to some extent. The most effective are the ‘intensive long-term programs’, which do promote full proficiency and literacy in both languages. Unfortunately such programs are extremely scarce in the U.S according to Thomas and Collier’s national study (1997) on over 700,000 language minority student records. Moreover, the “success” of bilingual programs was very often defined exclusively by how fast language minority students developed English proficiency and exited special programs (Thomas and Collier, 1997). Hence, schools served as an additional factor in replacing their native language with English. It is evident from our study that many Second generation or beyond respondents grew up speaking only English partly due to the school language policies.

2) Spanish-English Bilingualism – With 11.2 million illegal immigrants which make up 3.7% of the nation’s population and 5.2% of its labor force in March, 2010 (Pew Hispanic Center, 2011) – of whom the overwhelming majority is Spanish speaking – the Spanish language has become the second “unofficially” official language. According to the Pew Hispanic Center (2011), Mexicans make up the majority of the unauthorized immigrant population, 58%, or 6.5 million. Other nations in Latin America account for 23% of unauthorized immigrants, or 2.6 million. These 9.1 million illegal Hispanic/Latino immigrants in conjunction with the existing 50.5 million “legal” Hispanic/Latino population of the United States comprise a huge audience and an immensely attractive consumer market. The United States has granted amnesty to millions of illegal immigrants through different amnesty laws in the past. Prior to the Immigration and Reform Control Act of 1986, the United States government previously granted amnesty on a case by case basis. Since 1986, there have been seven mass amnesties and 6 million illegal aliens have received amnesty in the United States (Briggs, 2003).

The Spanish language has been established de facto as a second language not only in the private sector but the public and government sectors as well. That gives additional leverage to the Spanish speaking population – especially the first generation immigrants – by encouraging them “not to be eager” to learn English, thus, amplifying conservation of their ethnic identity, culture and “mother tongue”. This amplification contributes very positively to the socialization of their offspring as they would be growing up in a more “ethnic” environment. Knowing that the primary socialization takes place at home – we can easily speculate the magnitude of these conditions – something that hardly exists in the Greek America of today.

With the elevation of the Spanish language to a necessity – it will be more of an “inconvenience” for the Third or beyond generation to invest time in learning Greek – a language that most likely they would hardly use – when Spanish would evolve in an almost mandatory language – especially for those planning to make a successful career in the public or private sector.

At the same time, exogamous unions – in which Greek Americans have married members of other ethnic groups – continue to rise (Constantelos, 1999; Karapanagiotis, 2008). Although many non-Greek spouses have converted to the Greek Orthodox Church, their commitment to Greek culture and their capacity to convey it to their children are inherently problematical. This is evident in our study as the intermarriage rate has considerably progressed across generations.

After a brief resurgence in the late 1960s and the 1970s, enclave concentrations of Greeks within several major cities have resumed their long-standing decline as many of their residents relocate into suburban communities (Constantinou, 2007, p.259). In our study 77% of the respondents were located in a suburban area. This coincides with a study by Leontis (1997), which argued that Greeks in this country are more likely to reside scattered in the suburbs than in a city neighborhood or “Greek Towns”. Moreover, over 30% of the First (32.3%) and Second generation (37.3%) lived in an area where many other people of Greek descent lived. On the other hand, only 7.3% of the Third or beyond generation lived in an area with many other people of Greek descent. Nearly 5% of the Third or beyond generation lived in an area with no other people of Greek descent, as compared to 1.6% of the First generation and 3.2% of the Second generation. Our study further suggested that the ethnic composition is an important contributing factor to the loss and retention of culture core values. Living in a community with just a few people of Greek decent or even without other people of Greek descent was linked to a poorer Greek language skills with less positive attitudes and behav

Order Now

Order Now

Type of Paper
Subject
Deadline
Number of Pages
(275 words)