Health Essays – Binge Drinking Alcohol

Binge Drinking Alcohol

Question: With evidence for a new ‘culture of intoxication’, what are the current attitudes on binge drinking in UK society, and what are the possible causes for the current alcohol consumption patterns in contemporary society?

Introduction

The aim of this thesis is to answer the question: ‘With evidence for a new ‘culture of intoxication’, what are the current attitudes on binge drinking in UK society, and what are the possible causes for the current alcohol consumption patterns in contemporary society?’ This has been done by carrying out primary research in the form of a questionnaire in order to gain an insight into individuals’ drinking behaviours, and their attitudes towards binge drinking in western society.

This piece of work primarily focuses on different drinking patterns and attitudes amongst different social groups in society, and reasons behind these differences. Although social policy is touched upon during the thesis, this is primarily to put the thoughts and behaviours of the sample group into context, and the primary focus is on sociological factors behind the participants’ reactions to alcohol and binge drinking.

In recent years there has been an increasing public awareness and concern regarding binge drinking culture in the UK. Alcohol Concern (2003) stated that their research found that 40% of all male alcohol consumption takes the form of binge drinking sessions, while female binge drinking has increased significantly over the last decade.

The 1995 Government report, ‘Sensible Drinking’ altered the recommended drinking allowance for males and females from a weekly amount to a daily amount, resulting from concern over the UK binge drinking culture and the worry that ‘weekly consumption can have little relation to single drinking episodes and may indeed mask short term episodes which…often correlate strongly with both medical and social harm’. (Sensible Drinking, 1995 cited in Alcohol Concern, 2003)

Gofton (1990 cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 264) comments that socio economic restructuring during the 1980s has lead to changes in patterns of consumption, and the development of the British ‘lager lout’. Traditionally, drinking has been a male dominated, community based event but the decline in mixed age groups of male workers has meant that landlords cannot afford to rely on their traditional customer base and as a result drinking has developed into a pastime for both mixed and single sex groups; often age specific, participating in heavy sessional drinking during their free time at evenings and weekends. (Mintel, 1998 cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 265)

Measham and Brain (2005) argue that there have been four key transformations that have led to the recommodification of alcohol and the significant rise in sessional drinking. Firstly, a much wider range of alcoholic beverages have been produced in the last decade, to suit all tastes and budgets. There has also been an introduction of high strength alcoholic products, as well as increased strength of traditional alcoholic beverages such as beer and wine, by up to 50%.

Thirdly, there has been an increase in marketing and advertising of alcohol products to promote a culture of consumption and the need to purchase alcohol as a lifestyle marker and status symbol. The recent image overhaul of drinking establishments as trendy places, with drinks promotions to encourage greater alcohol consumption has also aided the transition towards a binge drinking culture.

The Government Licensing Act and other related policies have aimed to promote civilised drinking by addressing drunkenness and anti social behaviour related to drinking with harsher penalties against offenders and by introducing more liberal 24 hour licensing laws with the aim that having extended hours will encourage a more relaxed approach towards drinking as people will not rush to drink as much as they can before the 11 pm curfew.

Further sanctions have been placed on drinking establishments in an attempt to combat drink related disorder at the source by placing responsibility on licensed establishments to maintain order on their premises, to check for underage drinkers and to refuse to serve those who are already highly intoxicated. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 263)

A recent review of the licensing laws saw that overall, alcohol related crime rates had reduced as a more relaxed drinking climate has resulted from the introduction of 24 hour licensing laws. Culture Secretary, Andy Burnham commented, ‘Our main conclusion is that people are using the freedoms but people are not sufficiently using the considerable powers granted by the Act to tackle problems’. (Burnham, 2008 cited in Donaldson & Vina, 2008)

It would therefore appear that although a more positive image of alcohol consumption is being created in the UK, problems associated with binge drinking still exist and have not necessarily been addressed effectively through the change in licensing laws.

Alcohol Concern (2003) has noted that people in the UK have yet to understand fully the dangers of binge drinking. The majority view it as having a good time, a social norm and part of growing up. In the 1993 survey, findings showed that 42% of those who regularly binge drank recognised that it was harmful to their health but only 24% males and 31% females were motivated to change their drinking habits to improve their health.

Further to this the recent GENACIS study of gender and drinking found that the majority of individuals were willing to accept the negative experiences that sometimes occurred as a result of binge drinking in order to enjoy the mainly positive experiences of drunkenness.

To summarise, ‘drinking and drugs can be seen as indicators of socio economic and cultural change in the relationship between work, leisure and consumption. Not only do young people form their identities through consumption but the psychology of consumption is centred on the search for gratification, integration and identity formation’. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 276)

As such, it is understandable if binge drinking has developed as the latest moral panic based on established ideas about binge drinking and social disorder. It is therefore the aim of this study to uncover the attitudes and beliefs of those in current UK society regarding binge drinking and related drinking habits.

This piece of research into the attitudes and beliefs of people in the UK takes on an interpretive social perspective whereby studying a subject from a sociological perspective, it is important to consider that people are ‘active conscious beings’ (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 19) and it is necessary to take into account how people’s thoughts and beliefs influence their decisions of how they act as autonomous individuals interpreting the social world around them.

Weber believed that external causes on their own are not adequate explanations for people’s actions and that it is necessary to understand individual’s motivations and how they interpret the situation in order to fully understand social actions. (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 19)

As noted by Carey (2001), using the term binge drinking can cause some confusion as there are a number of definitions, largely based around amounts of alcohol consumed, which can cause the connotation of binge drinking to vary. For use in this survey, ‘binge drinking’ refers to excessive sessional consumption, which can lead to feelings of intoxication and drunkenness, whether intentional or unintentional. A qualitative definition of binge drinking has been used for this research as the disadvantages of allocating a certain quantitative unit of alcohol to the definition means that binge drinking becomes associated with levels of drinking dangerous to health and negative consequences.

It also fails to address that, although binge drinking frequently or over long periods of time can lead to a greater likelihood of problems or negative consequences as a result, that this is not always the case and that many people enjoy and have experienced positive effects from occasional binge drinking. (Weschler et al, 2000 cited in Carey, 2001; 285) By allocating quantitative measures it fails to address that binge drinking and drunkenness are subjective as different amounts of alcohol affect individuals at different levels, and perceived behavioural control is a primary influencing factor where alcohol consumption is concerned.

Research Methods

For this study, a combination of descriptive and explanatory primary research was carried out, in order to create an overview of people’s attitudes towards binge drinking, and individual drinking habits, as well as attempting to produce an explanation as to why people binge drink and why this is perceived as a social problem in society. (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 7)

A self completion questionnaire was decided upon as the most appropriate method of research as it was the most time and labour effective method of data collection. It is possible to obtain relatively extensive data from a large sample over a short time span using a questionnaire, which can increase the representativeness of the results making them more generalisable. (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 10)

After deciding on the issue of binge drinking, a limited literature review was carried out to ensure the subject to be studied was viable, and also in order to identify common themes and key questions to ask candidates. Literature included journal articles by Measham and Brain (2005), Ajzen and Madden (1986), and Johnston and White (2002).

A hypothesis was not developed as the research aimed to look for any general patterns in binge drinking behaviour and underlying reasons for the behaviour, and it was therefore felt that to create any specific hypotheses would create too much of a reductionist viewpoint from which to start the investigation.

As previously mentioned, a limited literature review was carried out in order to identify key issues that had occurred in previous research to develop an idea of significant areas to be covered in the questionnaire, but overall the aim was to maintain a broad perspective based around an interpretive reflection of findings and attempt at a causal analysis. The aim of the survey was to cover a variety of age groups with different socio economic circumstances, and different gender groups, to see if this affected attitudes towards binge drinking.

Approaching people directly and e-mailing copies of the questionnaire out to individuals developed a snowball sample whereby the questionnaire was passed on to others by initial participants and so the sample ‘snowballed’. Due to the vast number of people who use e-mail in contemporary society, and the convenience of using this medium of communication, it meant that e-mail was an especially viable method of contacting potential participants. (Sheehan & Hoy, 1999 in Seale, 2004; 107)

An initial draft questionnaire was developed using a variety of open and closed questions, primarily using closed questions to establish simple answers such as age, gender, income, etc with open questions primarily used as follow up to the closed questions to find out more detail and reason behind the answers selected in the closed questions. Some Likert scales were used to enable participants to express strength of opinion, which could be compared with other participants’ answers.

The advantage of closed questions is that they could easily be pre coded to enter directly onto the SPSS software programme to be analysed, while it can be difficult to organise answers of open questions into categories for post coding and comparing participants’ answers. (Moser & Kalton, 1971 in Seale, 2004; 83) While closed questions can verify their meaning by giving participants a selection of answers to choose from, and makes the survey easier for respondents to complete, it also results in a loss of spontaneity and detail in answers, and subjects may find that there is not always a category suitable for the answer they wish to give.

In contrast, although open questions are more time consuming to complete, they allow for more detailed and unusual answers to be given, as well as reducing demand characteristics enabling participants to give more open and less influenced answers. (Bryman, 2004; 147) For this reason, combinations of the two sorts of questions were used in order to enhance the effectiveness of the survey.

After a pilot run of the questionnaire that identified any faults, improvements were made and the final complete questionnaire was distributed amongst individuals with a range of ages. The disadvantage of snowball sampling is that it is much less systematic than other forms of sampling as it is based on the presumption that by selecting key individuals, other similar individuals will come forward who would make the sample representative, but this may not necessarily be the case.

On this occasion, gathering a sample based on this premise proved successful as no one age category had a significantly higher number of participants than any other. The overall constituency of the sample consisted of 14.3% Under 18s, 24.7% 18-30s, 19.5% 31-45s, 22.1% 46-60s, and 19.5% 61 and over.

In order to analyse the data the closed questions were pre coded and the open questions divided into common themes and post coded, then entered onto the SPSS software system in the form of figures and symbols which could be counted and added up, in order for the data to be analysed using a variety of statistical analysis techniques; primarily cross tabulation tables. (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 52)

After producing cross tabulation tables from the data collected, it was found that many of the supporting Chi Square Test results produced values bigger than 0.05, suggesting no significant relationships between the variables. This could have occurred for a number of reasons; the sample may not have been big enough, or large tables with many cells may not have had enough cases in each cell to generalise from even where the differences in percentages on the table appear to have quite a large difference.

In these cases, patterns could often be identified visually from looking at the tables based on the premise that a significant relationship could have been found if a larger sample had been used. However, caution in generalising these findings had to be taken based on the Chi Square results.

Secondary data in the form of literature reviews and statistics collected by others were also used to develop ideas, and also to verify findings from the primary research; more than one source of data is used to cross check findings and verify their validity. (Bryman, 2004; 545)

The reliability of the research is considerably higher than other methods that could have been used as questionnaires can be replicated with the exact questions and phrasing as many times as needed. However, with regard to the nature of some of the questions asked for this particular questionnaire about drinking habits which involve memory, reliability may be affected as people’s memories may not be accurate or may base their answers on their most recent drinking activity, so if asked another time may produce different answers. (Moser & Kalton, 1971 in Seale, 2004; 81)

Read also  Hypoglycaemia in a Term Infant Form Diabetic Mother

The validity of this research could be considered to build a relatively realistic picture of what is being studied as the validity of people’s answers could be checked by comparing what they say their attitudes towards alcohol consumption is, with their actual reported drinking behaviours.

A person is more likely to be open about their opinions and behaviours in writing than if asked to talk about them due to dissociation with the response. However, questionnaires can be affected by demand characteristics, particularly with closed questions, and people may pick what they believe to be the most socially acceptable rather than what they actually believe. (Moser & Kalton, 1971 in Seale, 2004; 75)

A further problem experienced with this questionnaire was that a number of questions were not completed on some questionnaires. It is not possible to know whether this is because participants accidentally missed the questions, or did not wish to disclose particular information. Further to this, some answers for questions such as asking people to select socially acceptable places to consume alcohol, it is possible that they selected those which reflected their own drinking habits, rather than what they thought the majority of society would view as socially acceptable. This may affect the validity of the final results.

This appears to be an ethically sound piece of research as all the participants were informed of the purpose of the research and what the disclosed information would be used for. It was made clear that their participation was voluntary, and they were therefore giving informed consent.

In an attempt to safeguard the privacy of research subjects, especially as the questionnaire required some personal information to be disclosed, all participants remained anonymous so that the information they disclosed remained confidential and could not be traced back to the individual. (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 13) It could be viewed as a concern that Under 18s were not asked to provide parental consent before completing the questionnaire, but due to the anonymity of the survey it was felt that it was unnecessary providing the participant themselves gave informed consent.

One way the overall research process could have been improved would have been to carry out more in depth preparatory investigations such as focus groups to discuss the issue with individuals and gain a deeper understanding of their perspective on the situation, and what they thought the key issues related to binge drinking were. A focus group could have helped create clearer guidelines for research and possibly aided the development of a hypothesis to create a more focused piece of research. (McNeill & Chapman, 2005; 32)

Alternative or additional methods of research could have been used to investigate the binge drinking culture in the UK. One option could have been to carry out an ethnography. This would allow the researcher to witness participant behaviour in their natural setting, without demand characteristics affecting the situation. However, for the purposes of this investigation it would have affected the representativeness of the population as certain places attract certain types of people.

Also, people will act differently in different situations and may be less likely to binge drink when at home than when out, but it would not be possible to observe this using an ethnography based in a single social setting. A further problem with ethnographies is that they can be the subjects of experimenter bias, as the experimenter must interpret the situation they are observing which is based on a subjective understanding of the situation. (Hammersley, 1991 in Seale, 2004; 243)

A further alternative method could have been to interview participants rather than using a questionnaire. This method could have collected factual and attitudinal data in greater depth than a questionnaire. However, interviewing is more time consuming as it involves recording and transcribing the conversations before it can be coded and analysed.

There is also the problem of interview bias, whereby the interviewee attempts to interpret what the interviewer wants from the situation based on their response to certain answers and the leading questions they ask. As mentioned before, the reduced anonymity of interviewing as a result of having to speak to someone face to face may affect the openness of the answers that participants give, and they may be less truthful or disclose less accurate information. (Bryman, 2004; 338)

Overall, it was felt that a questionnaire was the most appropriate method of data collection for this piece of research, providing the most effective means of collecting relevant data.

Culture of Consumption

‘Culture’ in this context is referring to the workings of society, from policy relating to the selling and consumption of alcohol, through to market influences and post traditional lifestyles and relationships which are leading to shared beliefs and values regarding binge drinking in contemporary society. Culture is important as it is through exposure to it within specific societal contexts that people develop their own thoughts and values regarding certain behaviours and what is socially acceptable.

According to the relevant Chi Square Test results, none of the Charts 1-4 in Appendix A showed a significant or reliable relationship between the variables. However, observations can be made from all the charts produced. Chart 1 which demonstrates the frequency of alcohol consumption of women with different levels of disposable income, shows that regardless of disposable income levels, the majority of women for each income category reported consuming alcohol more than once a week; 80% of those with a disposable income of less than £100 reported doing so, 100% of the £101-£400 per week doing so, and 66.7% of the over £400 per week.

This could suggest that although disposable income may restrict the frequency of alcohol consumption to some extent, overall a culture has developed whereby alcohol consumption is viewed as an important aspect of socialising, relaxation and leisure time.

Chart 3 showing frequency of alcohol consumption for the total sample with different levels of disposable income, also supports these findings. Interestingly, the £101-£400 per week disposable income group appears to consume alcohol the most frequently. This could be as a result of having a higher disposable income to spend on alcohol, yet more leisure time than those who earn more – who possibly work longer hours – in which to drink it.

From observing Chart 2 which shows the frequency of alcohol consumption of women with different sources of income, it can be seen that although the Chi Square has not shown a significant relationship between the two variables it would appear that whether income is earned – suggesting employment participation – seems to influence the frequency of alcohol consumption in women. 92.3% of those women who received income from employment consumed alcohol more than once a week, compared with 61.1% of those with an unearned income.

Reasons for this could be that women who work tend to drink more to reduce stress levels and as a method of relaxation, or it could be that those with an unearned income may have a lower disposable income to spend on alcohol, or that they feel the money is not theirs to spend on such luxuries so freely.

Alcohol related attitudes and behaviours which have been discussed so far in this study highlight changes which have occurred over time, not in isolation, but against a backdrop of economic, social and cultural change in transition from an industrial to a post industrial consumer society, and a ‘culture of consumption’. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 275)

In relation to alcohol Measham and Brain (2005) argue that the economy in the UK relies on the exploitation of hedonism. The UK nighttime economy, which has developed extensively over the last decade, thrives on the promotion of consumer excess and intoxication. Daniel Bell (1975, cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 275) argues that in contemporary society individuals have lost the influence of the traditional sources of structuring such as occupation and family, and as a result new sources of collective identity have been formed, based around the market and in terms of alcohol, around the night time hedonistic culture of consumption; individuals seek to construct their identity through consumer products.

This transition has facilitated greater opportunities for individuals to engage in hedonistic consumption, along with the decline in traditional norms and values that limited excessive consumption of alcohol. Cofield and Gofton (1994, cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 275) argue that amongst this hedonistic economy, drinking is no longer viewed only as a form of social integration, but more emphasis is placed on relaxation through the purchase of consumer products such as alcohol.

This idea is supported by James (Cited in Wessely, 1998) who views contemporary society as a ‘low serotonin society’ and claims that individuals in society are more depressed than ever before, and that the increased pace and competition of life has led to family breakdown and higher stress levels. James concludes that people in contemporary society are self obsessed and overly concerned with their emotional well being; as a result of this people are looking for methods of relaxation and escapism, alcohol of which is one, which in turn has lead to social changes and a culture of consumption in the pursuit of happiness. (Wessely, 1998)

This could be seen to support observations in chart 2 as those whose incomes come from paid employment drink more frequently, which is likely to be as a result of the stresses and strains of everyday life. Further to this it could be argued that the reason there is only a marginal difference between income and frequency of consumption is that it relates more to individual life circumstances and the stress levels in individuals’ lives rather than money available to spend on alcoholic products.

Chart 4, which shows differences in preferred alcohol beverage by different disposable income groups shows that no significant relationship exists between disposable income and preferred alcoholic beverage. However, it can be noted that overall the most popular alcohol beverage was wine/champagne, with 52.8% of the total sample selecting this as their preferred beverage. A reason for this may be that there are such a wide range of wines available to suit all tastes and budgets. 100% of those who had a disposable income of more than £400 per week chose wine/champagne as their preferred beverage.

This may be because expensive wine/champagne can be bought as a symbol of status in society, particularly if consuming in public places such as restaurants or high status bars. It can also be noted that 100% of those who selected alcopops as their preferred beverage were in the £100 per week or less disposable income category. This may be because generally alcopops are popular with younger drinkers, who are also likely to be those with a lower disposable income available to spend on alcohol.

The popular demand for wine as an alcoholic beverage is supported by Duffy’s research (1981; 200) which found that the consumption of alcohol products almost doubled between 1963 – 1979, particularly wine and spirits. He also comments that a reason for this is that the relative price of drink has declined which has enabled consumers to purchase alcohol in larger quantities. The British Medical Association has argued that raising the price of alcohol beyond inflation levels would aid the control of the problem of binge drinking, and that this could be achieved by introducing minimum prices for each type of alcoholic drink. (Louth, 2008)

However, it has been argued that increasing prices will not deter those who feel a ‘need’ for alcohol, just as increasing the prices of cigarettes has not stopped smokers buying them. It is an issue of personal demand over anything else. (Louth, 2008) Further to this, there are so many alcoholic drinks marketed by different producers that it would require an entirely new governmental system to control restrictions on the price and marketing of alcoholic beverages.

Duffy (1981; 202) investigated whether advertising and/or taxation contribute in any way to official alcohol control policy. The income elasticities were found to be 0.8 for beer, 1.7 for spirits and 2.2 for wine – these figures show the elasticity of demand in relation to income. These estimates of elasticity appear to correspond with the findings of chart 4, which suggests that wine is one of the luxury products which people tend to consume more of, especially in the privacy of their own homes, particularly as their disposable income increases.

The findings of Duffy’s research also suggest market demand for wine has increased at a greater rate than other alcoholic beverages, with consumption of wine increasing at a faster rate than the consumers’ relative income. (Duffy, 1981; 204) In 1970, on average individuals drank approximately seven litres a year of pure alcohol compared with an average of nine litres a year in contemporary society. Whereas the consumption of beer and spirits has remained relatively stable over a long period of time, the consumption of wine increased dramatically, representing over 80% of total alcohol consumption.

This would also suggest that increases in alcohol consumption cannot be directly related to anti social behaviour as many of those middle class individuals, reflected in the findings relating to income and preferred alcoholic beverage, will not be seen creating disturbances and expressing anti social behaviour in public spaces. Many prefer to consume alcohol within the privacy of their own homes. (Louth, 2008)

Yet Government statistics for middle class individuals who participate in habitual wine drinking show that they are consuming enough alcohol to cause considerable damage to their health, and government research has suggested this hidden drinking culture is also problematic in terms of putting pressure on the NHS through alcohol related hospital admissions, but largely ignored as it occurs behind closed doors. The Public Health Minister, Dawn Primarolo commented ‘Most of these (admissions) are not young people; they are ‘everyday’ drinkers who have drunk too much for too long.’ (Primarolo, 2007 cited in Boseley, 2007)

Duffy also found that the estimated elasticities for advertising of all alcoholic beverages were low, especially for wine. The assumption here is that there are so many adverts for other alcohol products competing against each other and against other products in general, that they cancel each other out and have relatively little impact upon the consumer. However, their continuous presence is likely to contribute to the popular cultural belief of alcohol consumption as an integral part of social life in western society. (Duffy, 1981; 205)

Read also  Early Attempts on Gene Modification

It can also be noted that the shots category was not selected by anyone as a preferred beverage and as such is not shown on the chart. However, this may be because shots are not drunk as a primary beverage on a night out but as a supplement, or as a chaser, in order to catalyse the development into a state of drunkenness. Measham and Brain’s research (2005) found that although shots and shooters were not considered a primary product to consume on a night out drinking, they were becoming an established part of a night out binge drinking.

They were mainly drunk as a cheap addition to a night’s alcohol consumption with 40-60% of participants in the study reporting consuming at least one shot on a night out. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 270) The main reasons found for consumers purchasing shots were to quicken the process of achieving drunkenness, and as a cheap way of making an inexpensive gesture towards friends by buying them shots too – they are cheaper than a typical round of drinks for those who cannot afford to pay out more.

The increasing popularity of ‘doing’ shots has created concerns regarding issues of responsible drinking patterns. By consuming shots alongside long drinks it increases the speed and quantity of alcohol consumed as shots tend to be ‘downed in one’ as well as encouraging people to mix their drinks, which can heighten the state of intoxication.

Drinking patterns such as these can be seen to have implications regarding the corporate responsibility of the retail drinks industry with regard to sales of alcohol – the quantity of sales and method of service, and also the alleged government policy which promotes and supports only civilised and responsible drinking in the UK. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 271)

Measham (2004, cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 271) notes that the UK has a significantly different pattern of purchasing and consuming alcohol in relation to other countries such as those in Europe and North America. In the UK the purchasing of alcohol is based around customers approaching the bar and waiting to be served, whereas in the previously mentioned countries alcohol purchase is based around table service that prolongs the cycle of consumption, especially during busy times of day.

It appears that the overall ‘culture of consumption’ has developed towards drinking as a primary aspect of socialising and relaxation. Disposable income appears to have little impact on the frequency of alcohol consumption, as there are different beverages available to suit all budgets. A more important factor seems to be stress levels and free time. This has been supported by the recent moral panic created by the media regarding middle class excessive drinking – those who most likely have stressful jobs with a lot of responsibility, but more time to drink than even higher earners.

It can also be noted that women who work consume alcohol more frequently than women who do not. With more women participating in the labour market in contemporary society it may affect the consumption frequency of younger generations of women who have higher disposable incomes and also use alcohol for relaxation in their hectic lives.

Further to this, as traditional social structures break down, younger generations are looking for other ways to form identities through consumer products such as alcohol in order to facilitate integration and acceptance into today’s society, as well as conforming to the demands of the hedonistic night time economy.

Age related drinking habits

Chart 1 in Appendix B, reporting frequency of drunkenness for different age groups, suggests that binge drinking to the point of drunkenness decreases with age. For Under 30s the highest percentage of participants in those categories report frequency of drunkenness as being ‘more than once a month’, while the 31- 60 age group’s most popular response was ‘less than once a year’, with only 10% less (44.4%) reporting drunkenness as ‘once a year or more but never more than once a month’, and the Over 60 age group having 85% of their responses in ‘less than once a year’.

The results that stand out the most are that only participants in the Under 30 age group reported getting drunk more than once a month, with a significant proportion (23.3%) placing themselves in that category. From the overall sample, the most popular response for frequency of drunkenness was ‘less than once a year’ (45%).

The Chi Square Test appeared to produce a significant figure of 0.00 which would suggest a relationship between age and frequency of drunkenness except that 33.3% of cells had an expected count of less than 5, meaning Chi Square Test results are not reliable. However, the visual results from this would suggest that overall binge drinking culture in the UK might not be as prevalent as currently believed although it is hard to identify whether frequency of drunkenness declines with age, or whether different generations have different attitudes towards drinking and those who are over 60 have never experienced an extensive binge drinking culture in their youth.

Chart 2 in Appendix B shows those who have and those who have never been drunk in each age group and displays that for those under 60 years of age, the majority have experienced a state of drunkenness at some point, whilst there is a significant drop for those 61 years and over who have experienced drunkenness – only 46.7% compared with 90% under 30s and 84.4% in the 31- 60 age group.

It is therefore a possibility that to a certain degree, frequency of drunkenness does decline with age but those over 60 may have grown up in a generation where binge drinking was not considered an integral part of entertainment and socialising as a young adult. This relates to the idea discussed in the Culture of Consumption chapter, that as traditional structures of identity have disintegrated they have been replaced with a drinking culture as a method of creating personal identities.

The Pearson Chi Square provides a significant figure of 0.002 that suggests the relationship between age and whether a person has ever been drunk is generalisable. However, the Lambda measurement only gives a value of 0.067, which suggests only a weak relationship. None the less it is possible to see from looking at the cross tabulation table that as age increases the number of people who have experienced drunkenness seems to decline.

It is also possible to see from Chart 3 in Appendix B, which shows the difference between those who have and have not experienced drunkenness for gender as well as age, that at one end of the spectrum more under 18 girls have been drunk (83.3%) than under 18 boys (50%) yet for 61 and over females, only 16.7% have ever been drunk compared with 66.7% of men in the same age category. The Chi Square Test indicates that the results are not reliable for males or females, and would not be generalisable for males, yet for women the generalisability of results could be investigated further as a significant figure of 0.001 was produced. However, the Lamba value produced was only 0.444 which does not suggest a particularly strong relationship between the two variables. This would suggest that the stereotyped gender image of male binge drinking is disintegrating in post traditional society.

Kemm (2003) used statistics from the General Household Survey from 1978 – 1998 to create a cross sectional study on drinking behaviour from which cohorts could be constructed. The results showed that at all ages in all cohorts there were a higher number of females than males who were light or non drinkers, with numbers being lowest around the 20 year old age mark and increasing to reach a peak at around the 80 year old mark with approximately 40% males being light or non drinkers, and 65% females. (Kemm, 2003; 143)

In a cross sectional analysis it was also found that although drinking levels decreased with age, for each cohort it was found that the numbers of males likely to be heavy drinkers decreased whilst the number of females likely to be heavy drinkers increased, although only with a slight tendency. (Kemm, 2003; 144) Results also showed that drinking behaviours for both males and females were similar in any cohort, but the most significant difference in findings was that for cohorts of men born before 1927-32, and for women born after 1927-32, the earlier cohorts were less likely to be heavy drinkers than the later cohorts, and also that the difference between male and female drinking levels has decreased in more recent cohorts with higher numbers of both sexes participating in heavy drinking behaviour. (Kemm, 2003; 145)

The reason for this cohort effect in older generations could be attributed to Social Interaction Theory. Neve et al (1993, cited in Gilhooly, 2005; 277) claim that as a result of Social Interaction Theory that ‘interaction between people in interconnected social networks leads to reciprocal influence of drinking behaviour throughout society.’ Although drinking behaviours may change as a result of external factors such as change in the price of beverages, a conflict between those who diverge from the ‘norm’ and those who do not leads to a gradual convergence of the two perspectives in order to meet in the middle and create a new order of social acceptability known as a ‘collectivity of drinking cultures’. (Skog, 1985 cited in Gilhooly, 2005; 277)

For those in older cohorts who may have grown up around the time of the Second World War when access to alcohol would have been rationed and people had a lower disposable income, the ‘collectivity of drinking cultures’ created would have led to a lower consumption of alcohol to be regarded as the socially acceptable amount and binge drinking would not have been viewed as a priority.

Further to this, Gilhooly also comments that ageing effect rather than cohort effect may alter the drinking habits of individuals. As people enter retirement, for the majority this is associated with a decrease in income and therefore a decrease in the quantity of alcohol consumed. However, with more free time available it may mean that older people consume alcohol more frequently (little and often).

Gilhooly’s results showed that frequency of alcohol consumption amongst individuals in Scotland increased with age. However, it must also be noted that due to the higher numbers of teetotal individuals as a result of the cohort effect, a polarisation occurs with age between those who drink frequently and those who do not consume alcohol at all. (Gilhooly, 2005; 275)

Research has also been carried out regarding reported high levels of intoxication and binge drinking amongst undergraduate students (primarily in the 18-30 age group). Gill (2002) carried out a literature review and compared the results of 18 studies investigating drinking habits of students at UK universities over the last 25 years. Delk & Meilman (1996, cited in Gill, 2002; 116) reported that 62.6% of all students admitted to binge drinking in the previous fortnight, with 11% having done it more than 5 times. Norman at el (1998, cited in Gill, 2002; 116) also found that 64% males and 32.5% females reported binge drinking at least once per week.

For many individuals in this age group, binge drinking is viewed as a normal pattern of alcohol consumption. (Gill, 2002; 119) Reasons given by students for consuming alcohol included drinking for pleasure, enjoying the taste and being sociable. (West et al, 1990 cited in Gill, 2002; 114) Despite this, the majority of those who had participated in binge drinking also reported negative effects of broken or damaged friendships, doing something they had later regretted and 10% of females had been assaulted. (Gill, 2002; 117)

This consolidates the results reported in Chart 1 of Appendix B, that the younger generations in today’s society experience a much more prevalent binge drinking culture. The main difference was reported by File et al (1994, cited in Gill, 2002; 113) who found that there was a strong relationship between the ethnicity of students and their abstention from alcohol; 54% male Asian students and 65% Asian female students reported never drinking, primarily as a result of religious and cultural beliefs.

From a different perspective, Alcoholics Anonymous (2007) has addressed the issue that while teenagers and younger generations are the group primarily focused upon for problem drinking, people are failing to turn their attention to the fact that in recent years more older people are seeking help from addiction treatment centres. In the last 10 years, alcohol addiction patients aged over 65 years have increased from 13% to 20%. The GGZ Nederland Report (cited in Alcohols Anonymous Reviews, 2007) has claimed that, contradictory to what people might think, one thing that the elderly and young have in common is time, and that elderly people are likely to drink even more frequently than the young.

This could be related back to James’s ‘Low Serotonin Society’ that the elderly drink as a form of escapism from ill health and possible loneliness. This highlights the necessity to address the way society as a whole approaches alcohol. (Revill, 2005)

Overall it appears that although there are a minority of older people in society who experience alcohol related problems, binge drinking and drunkenness has a tendency to decrease with age as a result of changing circumstances over the life course. It appears that cultural influences in past times have affected the attitudes and behaviours of earlier age cohorts who had less exposure to a binge drinking culture as a result of different societal influences during their youth.

However, it is easy to see that attitudes and behaviours have been changing in more recent cohorts and differences between gender groups in relation to binge drinking are also changing in younger generations.

Gender Differences in Alcohol Consumption

It can be seen that charts 1-3 in Appendix C have no significant relationship between the variables, as shown by the corresponding Chi Square tests. However, Chart 1 reporting how many of the sample have or have not experienced drunkenness for each gender group, does show that a similar percentage of male and female participants had experienced drunkenness with slightly more females (79.5%) having done so than males (78.1%).

The fact that there was no significant relationship between gender and frequency of drunkenness (shown in Chart 2), or gender and enjoyment of drunkenness (shown in Chart 3) may reflect changes in gender role attitudes in relation to binge drinking.

Although there appear to be relationships for charts 4 & 5 highlighting views of acceptability of male/female binge drinking and the associated reasons given for levels of justification, 44.4% of cells on both relating Chi Squares had expected counts of less than 5 and the findings were therefore unreliable. However, comparing charts 4 & 5, it can be noted that for both male and female binge drinking the most popular views were that it was ‘highly unacceptable or never acceptable’ with 66.1% recipients in this category for male binge drinking and 68.3% in this category for female binge drinking.

Read also  Nature of Cortical Bone

The main difference between views of male and female binge drinking was that the most common justification given for the lack of acceptability of male binge drinking was anti social behaviour such as violence and aggression, which affects others, whereas for women the most common reason given was that it was important to remain in control at all times, as women are considered more at risk from physical and health dangers, and also because it is undignified and unladylike.

Chart 6 demonstrating the significance of relationship status to frequency of drunkenness in women, shows that the highest percentage (47.4%) of the female sample population who were not in a relationship were in the drunk ‘more than once a month’ category compared with the highest percentage of those who were in a relationship reporting getting drunk ‘less than once a year’ (66.7%).

However, it must be noted that the Chi Square showed that there was not a significant or reliable relationship between the two variables, even though visually the cross tabulation table would suggest that those who are not in a relationship are more inclined to get drunk more frequently.

A contradictory viewpoint by Johnston et al (1977, cited in Parker et all, 1980; 45) found that ‘married women who are employed have significantly higher rates of both problem and heavier drinking than either single working women or housewives. No similar relationship occurs for men, raising the distinct possibility that this type of non traditional role for women leads to increased risk of alcoholism.’

Parker et al investigated further the effects of gender role on drinking habits. 795 men and women aged over 18 who consumed alcohol more than once a month were interviewed. However, their findings did not support those of Johnston et al in their earlier study. They did find though that although marital status does not relate to the drinking patterns of women, it does relate to the drinking patterns of men. They believe that an explanation of this is that men tend to drink more when young, free and single, but frequency and amount of alcohol consumption decline once married as they have more responsibilities such as child care, and it is seen as less acceptable and socially irresponsible.

Further to this, many women experience these constraints in a greater variety of social contexts, which may affect drinking acceptability throughout their life course meaning that their drinking habits may not change as drastically over time. (Parker et al, 1980; 46) However, Chart 1 in Appendix B showed that younger age cohorts are inclined to drink more, and Chart 3 in Appendix B showed that this is especially true for women, which may show how gender role attitudes have changed.

These findings also relate to the findings in the Age chapter relating to decrease in alcohol consumption and drunkenness with age as a result of personal circumstances and increased responsibilities in adulthood.

Gender role attitudes assess individual’s beliefs regarding what are appropriate male and female behaviours. (McHugh & Frieze, 1997; 4) Larsen and Long (1988) investigated whether the strong feminist movements of the 1960s had affected the traditional gender role attitudes of 484 undergraduate students. (Larsen & Long, 1988; 1) Traditional sex role ideology views women’s roles as maternal and care giving roles, whereas men should work and provide for their families.

More egalitarian views of sex roles suggest that the difference between men and women is only physical, and psychological differences are a result of social construct; in effect men and women should not be discriminated against adopting particular roles as they should be allocated based on ability and not based on gender. The findings suggested that females had a more egalitarian attitude towards sex roles than males.

A reason for this is that males may feel threatened or intimidated by the increasing equality of women as by maintaining a traditional sex role approach it allows men to maintain dominance over women. (Larsen & Long, 1988; 9) The results also showed that social events in the 60s and 70s and the feminist movement had affected attitudes towards gender roles, particularly in women, which may be a reason as to why there has been a declining difference between male and female drinking habits in younger cohorts, reflected in the transition from traditional structural formations of gender identity to the modern day ‘culture of consumption’.

An example of the effects of the changing gender role attitudes is the newly derived ‘ladette’ social representations. It is argued that the ‘laddish’ attitudes these females have adopted is a result of the increasing egalitarian attitudes towards men and women, and the portrayal by the media of aggressive images of women. (Muncer et al, 2001; 33)

Adler (175, cited in Muncer et al, 2001; 34) developed a sociological masculinity hypothesis claiming that as a result of the increase in women’s entry into the workforce, which was originally viewed as a masculine gender role, it has created a slippery slope with more gender blind opportunities opening up to women allowing them to adopt more masculine behavioural attitudes such as those who adopt ‘ladette’ roles, who hold less traditional gender role attitudes than their peers. (Muncer et al, 2001; 34)

The British media have commented that new cultural images of ‘laddish’ women has entered the media as a result of increasingly masculine attitudes of young women, who are believed to have adopted this lurid, confrontational and binge drinking culture of working class males. (Muncer et al, 2001; 35) Patricia Pearson commented:

‘Girls today do not only live in a generally more violent society, but their role models are more assertive, in every realm from female rock stars to prime time TV, to movie stars. The capacity to be physically forceful and volubly angry is much more evident to these girls, as it is to young women in western nations’. (Pearson, 1998 cited in Muncer et al, 2001; 36)

However, in contrast to this it has also been found that despite the results of this thesis, previous research has suggested differences between male and female drinking attitudes and behaviours. (Hassan, & Shiu, 2007; 318) For example, Wilsnack et al (2000, cited in Hassan & Shiu, 2007; 318) in their research found that men are 10% more likely to drink alcohol than women, and in larger quantities.

Also, more women were found to report ill effects as a consequence of alcohol consumption even with the same blood concentrate level as men, which may act as a deterrent to the frequency and levels of alcohol women consume.

Hassan & Shiu (2007) asked 110 female and 107 male students to fill out a self-completion questionnaire at different times and different locations across a Scottish university campus, in order to investigate gender differences in drinking attitudes and behaviours in relation to the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Their results showed that in relation to low risk single occasion drinking, women were likely to exceed the recommended limit approximately 1.5 times per week, and twice per week for males.

Examining the male and female results individually found that 38% females exceeded the limit at least twice a week compared with 58% males. In relation to the Theory of Planned Behaviour, significant gender differences were discovered, but also similarities. In this context perceived behavioural control was not a significant factor compared with attitude and subjective norm, in adopting specific behavioural intentions, but the theory of planned behaviour model did not perform as well for predicting female behavioural intentions.

However, the reason perceived behavioural control may have appeared to be an insignificant factor may have been that because so many participants viewed their behavioural control as strong and took it as a given norm they did not view it as an issue. (Hassan & Shiu, 2007; 323)

Results also showed that the strongest indicator of female intentions of keeping within the recommended limits for low risk single occasion drinking was normative beliefs, as well as behavioural beliefs and attitudes. This therefore suggests that subjective norm is the largest contributing factor towards female drinking habits overall. Research also showed that, compared with males, females were more likely to intentionally remain within the recommended limits for low risk single occasion drinking and had a more positive attitude towards it.

The female participants also reported paying greater attention to government media campaigns regarding binge drinking, which may have been more likely to influence their drinking habits compared to males. (Hassan & Shiu, 2007; 324) This does suggest that female attitudes and behaviour towards binge drinking reflect more greatly the common belief demonstrated in findings of this study suggesting that female binge drinking is not acceptable as women are more at risk of physical harm and poor health as a result of excessive alcohol consumption.

Overall, findings in relation to gender and binge drinking reported decreases in differences between the attitudes and behaviours of males and females towards binge drinking. There were still some differences but this could be accounted for the fact that gender roles are changing but are still in a transitional phase with many still holding traditional views of gender roles. The emergence of non-traditional female gender roles (married women in employment) and new identities such as ‘ladettes’, do seem to have led to higher rates of binge drinking.

Views of acceptability of male and female binge drinking found in the sample do not seem to be reflected in drinking behaviours, which appears to result from perceived behavioural control whilst drinking which can lead to intentional binge drinking, as well as it being perceived as the subjective norm, particularly for younger generations. This aspect will be discussed further in the Determined Drunkenness chapter.

Determined Drunkenness

One noticeable result from the research findings can be seen in Chart 1 in Appendix D showing a frequency table and bar chart demonstrating the percentage of individuals in the sample who have ever been drunk. This is that although a vast majority of participants (78.2%) had been drunk at some point in their lives, almost 20% had not. These findings reflect the situation that for many in today’s society, it is acceptable to become intoxicated to the point of drunkenness; however, this is not true for all – particularly those from earlier age cohorts as discussed already.

Further to this, although these findings demonstrate that a large majority have experienced a state of drunkenness, therefore having consumed themselves in a binge drinking culture, it is necessary to look deeper into whether drunkenness is achieved intentionally, and their reasons for doing so. It is also necessary to look at how acceptable those who do/do not drink view binge drinking amongst others in society.

Measham & Brain (2005) claim that the reasons the majority of people have experienced drunkenness results from their change in attitudes and what is seen as a socially acceptable state of intoxication for alcohol drinkers. The UK’s National Alcohol Strategy Unit (2004, cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 268) comments that ‘for many people in England today, going out to get drunk has become part of a “good night out”…Drinking is often viewed as an end in itself, and public drunkenness is socially accepted, if not expected’. Evidence of a new ‘culture of intoxication’ has become prevalent in more recent times reflecting the change in attitudes of the majority. (Measham & Brain, 2005)

Measham and Brain (2005) conducted fieldwork research during 2004 at three different nightlife venues in Manchester, which incorporated a measure to assess intoxication related attitudes and behaviour of 351 respondents on a night out in Manchester by using a five point Likert type scale to measure ‘actual’ and ‘desired’ state of intoxication of participants. On average, females reported having already drunk 5.7 units of alcohol, and males 9.6 units, and on average respondents reported current levels of drunkenness as 1.8 out of 5, whilst their intentions were on average to experience a level of drunkenness of 3.8 out 5 by the end of the evening. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 272)

One significant finding of the research was that at an assessed level of only 1.8 out of 5 for drunkenness, women were on the verge of exceeding the unit limit of 6 to be classed as binge drinkers according to UK standard, and males had already exceeded the limit of 8 units, with an intended night of drinking still ahead of them. This result creates implications for the Government’s claim that binge drinkers are a minority of anti social individuals within the UK population, who should not be used as an excuse to deter legislation of more liberal 24 hour drinking laws for the enjoyment of the majority who do drink sensibly.

A further subjective observation was that although the majority of respondents were classed as binge drinkers, only the minority would be viewed as anti social as the majority ended the drinking session by returning home without committing criminal offences or causing harm to themselves or others. (Measham & Brain, 2005)

Despite the research findings that the majority of participants intended to get drunk, the fact that the average intended state of drunkenness was assessed at 3.8 out of 5 suggests that their intended state of drunkenness was not completely uncontrolled, and that personal limits of alcohol consumption were acknowledged. Measham (2002, cited in Measham & Brain, 2005; 273) describes this scenario as ‘controlled loss of control’.

This state of intoxication is designed to enable drinkers to enjoy the experience of intoxication whilst still acknowledging social responsibility for themselves and others. The main motivations indicated for ‘controlled loss of control’ over complete loss of control were concerns over personal safety – getting home safely, health – minimising hangovers, security – to avoid becoming involved in potentially threatening situations, and for those who chose to abstain from drinking a greater desire to drive than drink – for convenience, and also for the status associated with driving a high performance car for those who owned one. (Measham & Brain, 2005; 274) It is also important to point out that the research defined no significance between gender, age or fieldwork location.

Order Now

Order Now

Type of Paper
Subject
Deadline
Number of Pages
(275 words)